Saturday, July 19, 2014

Perspectives






     Cognitive Psychology is solicitous with advances in the study of memory, language processing, perception, problem solving, and thinking.  Whereas, behaviorist theories are theoretically different from the founding theories of cognitive psychology as behaviorism examines how, not why, learning occurs.  History records three of the most ardent behaviorists as John B. Watson, Edward C. Tolman, and B. F. Skinner.  Although each theorist began with a fundamentally similar approach, each rejected his introspective forefathers to advance the assumptions of behaviorism.  However, psychology will always maintain all their significances “[f]or under the surface of contradiction, lies similarity” (Jordan, 2010, para. 1).

Behaviorist Perspectives

John B. Watson (1878-1958)

     Watson was born into the dysfunctional family of an alcoholic father and an overly religious mother (Goodwin, 2008).  Although he was a presumptuous child, Watson’s youthful ways improved when at 16 he began attending Furman University.  With the inspiration of one of his professors Watson transferred to the University of Chicago where he studied philosophy and psychology; however, he was not enamored with the introspective point of view of the time, but “accepted the general precepts of functionalist psychology” (Goodwin, 2008, p. 338).  Watson obtained his Ph.D. in psychology in 1903 and began working for the university thereafter.  His early research involved animal behavior as associated with kinesthetic (muscle) senses in learning while working with white rats and mazes.  Because of the invasive nature of his studies (e.g., surgical removal of the eyes, middle ears, and olfactory bulbs in rat’s), it was written that Watson was a butcher of white rats for the curiosity of science (Goodwin, 2008).  In 1908 Watson took a position at Johns Hopkins in Baltimore, Maryland, as full professor providing him with control of his own laboratory.

     At Hopkins his research helped him to articulate his belief that human behavior should be studied under exacting laboratory conditions, much like his animal behavior studies with the article Psychology as a Behaviorist Views It (1913).  The following year (1914) he published his Behavior: An Introduction to Comparative Psychology where he argued conditioned responses is the ideal atmosphere for psychological study.  It was not until 1918 that Watson’s most infamous research began; Little Albert.  When to opportunity arose to study infants, Watson moved forward with his behaviorist principles of conditioned emotional responses of which he and his assistants could identify three: fear, rage, and love.  Little Albert, however, was the infant who Watson used to demonstrate his theories of conditioned behaviors; instilling phobic responses in an otherwise normal nine month old, much like Pavlov’s research.  Although there is some question regarding the ethics of the Little Albert research, the results (that behaviors are a process of learning) made behaviorism a leading perspective in American psychology (Goodwin, 2008).  Watson lost his position at Johns Hopkins shortly after the Little Albert study went to print in 1920, but quickly moved into the corporate world of advertising where he put his behaviorist perspective to use.  He continued working with behaviorist philosophies but not with rat’s, instead he moved into the area of child rearing; with that, no essay on Watson is complete without his most famous quote, from his book Behaviorism (1924/1930), where he states:

"Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train him [or her] to become any type of specialist I might select - doctor, lawyer, artist, merchant-chief, and, yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations, and race of his ancestors" (p. 104).

     Watson’s viewpoint eventually became known as classical S-R (stimulus- response) behaviorism or the first phase of the behavioral revolution (Moore, 2011).    

Edward C. Tolman (1886-1959)

     Tolman was the second son of a Massachusetts business executive and his Quaker wife.  Expected to follow in their father’s footsteps, both boys chose to attend Massachusetts Institute of Technology.  Edward graduated from MIT in 1911 with a Bachelor’s of Science degree in Electrochemistry; however, young Tolman decided to pursue his interest in philosophy, which began during his senior year when he took an interest in the work of William James (Goodwin, 2008).  Tolman’s interest in philosophy and psychology encouraged him to enroll at Harvard where he became enthralled with psychology after reading Watson’s book Behavior: An Introduction to Comparative Psychology; earning his psychology degree in 1918.  Although Tolman did not immediately declare himself a behaviorist, his introduction to Kurt Koffka and to gestalt psychology began to impress upon him the behaviorist theology.  Tolman believed, however, that behavior was a composite between the teachings of his MIT professor, Yerkes, and that of Watsons S-R framework.  He called his new behaviorist perspective “comparative psychology” (Goodwin, 2008, p. 365).  Like Watson, Tolman worked on animal maze research to develop his unique form of behaviorism.  Tolman took a faculty position at Berkeley in 1918 where he continued his research and published several articles, such as A New Formula for Behaviorism (1922) and Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men (1932).  Furthermore, Tolman proposed that molar behaviors (muscle memory) are purposive, leading to the formation of cognitive maps and latent learning.  Tolman (1948) explains it:

"The stimuli which are allowed in are not connected by just simple one-to-one switches to the outgoing responses. Rather the incoming impulses are usually worked over and elaborated in the central control room into a tentative cognitive-like map of the environment. And it is this tentative map, indicating routes and paths and environmental relationships, which finally determine what responses, if any, the animal will finally make" (p. 192).

     Tolman’s work with rats running mazes continued with and without reward (e.g., food, and water), reinforcing his theory of latent learning, which suggests that learning occurs even in the absence of reward.

B.F. Skinner (1904-1990)

     Skinner began his career at New York’s Hamilton College, graduating in 1926.  During a brief hiatus after graduation, he became intrigued by the writings of Watson and Pavlov and soon found himself enrolled in Harvard’s psychology program (Goodwin, 2008).  Although Skinner became an ardent follower of Watson and the behaviorist perspective, he believed that behaviors are learned dependent on one’s environment and experiences, whether they be positive or negative.  He called this form of learning operant conditioning.  Furthermore, he suggested that changing negative behaviors is possible through reinforcement principles, earning him the honor of becoming a radical behaviorist; Skinner himself explains:

“I don’t believe I coined the term radical behaviorism, but when asked what I mean by it, I have always said, ‘the philosophy of a science of behavior treated as a subject matter in its own right apart from internal explanations, mental or physiological’” (Skinner, 1989, p. 122 as restated by Moore, 2011, p. 456).

     Skinner was also responsible for introducing the operant learning theory, however.  Furthermore, he discloses that type S conditioning, as stated in the Pavlovian model, is an identified stimulus (e.g., food, and a tone), he believed the type R conditioning is the operant condition (Goodwin, 2008).  Skinners main focus was on how someone’s environment shapes his or her behaviors and he describes the process in his 1953 book Science and Human Behavior.  Skinner, like Watson and Tolman, worked with rats (and pigeons), which helped him to control his experimental environment.  Choosing to further the theories of behavior, Skinner designed an operant chamber allowing him to record subject behavior and the stimuli creating it.

     Because of Skinners automation processes, it was quite by accident that he discovered extinction; a gradual decline in response rate when reinforcement is withdrawn, resulting inevitably in no response.  This led Skinner to work with a schedule of reinforcement suggesting that patterns of behavior vary as a function of various reinforcement schedules.  Like Watson, Skinner published his ideas on child-rearing in his 1948 fictional book Walden Two reflecting his desires for a kinder, gentler world order (Goodwin, 2008).  Although he did not elaborate further on child behaviors, Skinners ideas of operant conditioning dominated American psychology for decades.

Contrasting Perspectives

     Although it is clear that behaviorism has become an accepted norm by American psychologists, it has within its structure many perspectives.  Watsonian behaviorism or classic behaviorism, as it is known, suggests that behavior is a learning process and can be altered by modifying ones conditions.  Tolman maintains that the presence or absence of reward affects behavioral learning, and that learning continues without the knowledge of it occurring.  Furthermore, his theories take into account cognitive mapping processes within a subject; whereas, Skinner rejects the inner causes of behavior placing his theories on observable behaviors.  He also suggests, unlike Tolman that continually rewarded behaviors will extinguish eventually, but he still believes that behaviors are derived by motivation. 

Applied Behaviorism

     Behaviorism as a perspective studies the different processes of learning and the effects of the environment on how, when, and why learning takes place.  In the 21st century “the assumptions, methods, and practices of behaviorism make it an ecological model of learning” (Daly, III, n.d., para. 8).  Behaviorist perspectives have been applied in learning models within the educational system, teaching adaptive behavioral skills (e.g., self-help, safety behaviors, vocational training), and within behavioral treatment facilities (e.g., antecedents, reinforcer’s, and punishers) to modify the environmental stimuli providing more effective treatment designed to the individual not the facility.  As demonstrated by Skinner, behaviorism is also applied in the corporate world (e.g., advertising, architecture, ergonomics, employee relations) to enable individuals to function in his or her surroundings and be more productive.

     Although psychology is the science of the mind and its processes as related to human and animal behavior, behaviorism, specifically looks at the relationship between the behavior and the environment.  Watson believed that behaviors can and are the response to one’s environment and by changing his or her stimulus so to changes the response.  Tolman’s form of behaviorism takes into account Watson’s theories and goes a step further suggesting that behaviors once learned are kept as cognitive maps, which are then brought forth when needed.  The most radical behaviorist is undoubtedly B. F. Skinner who believed that behaviors are observable experiences to specific stimuli. Regardless of their differences their foundations remain the same, behaviorist psychology can be adapted to bring practical techniques to any situation.  The last century of behaviorism has brought significant insight into human behavior, perhaps the next will allow man to discover even more about human psychology.   


 

No comments:

Post a Comment