Sunday, October 19, 2014

Learning and Cognition




     The most studied concept in psychology is that of how humans acquire knowledge; learning and cognition.  From the earliest recordings of Plato and Aristotle man has grappled with the nature of knowledge.  However, it was not until the sixteenth century that philosophy took a decisive turn when Rene’ Descartes (1596-1650) argued for the separation between mind and body, thus giving credence to the minds ability to direct the actions and behavior of the body (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009).  Furthermore, the cognitive processes that enable learning play an intricate role in what and how people acquire knowledge. 
Definition of Learning
     Throughout history the definition of learning has transformed from a philosophical thought process to the “comprehension, knowledge, or understanding that is gained from experience” (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009, p. 1).  While still valid, the most popular definition is that of Gregory A. Kimble (1917-2006).  He suggests, according to Olson and Hergenhahn (2009) that “learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior or in behavioral potentiality that results from experience…” (p. 6).  This definition leaves the type of experience open for interpretation, however.
Behavioral Role of Learning
     A behavior is the reaction or response as a result of environmental and psychological stimuli, but according to theorists that does not mean that specific behavior was learned (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009).  However, the intervening variables (experiences) that take place in order for someone to learn are vast and have the capability of causing a relatively permanent change in behavior; thus it can be said that learning takes place.  The difficulty lies in identifying these variables, thus theorists use behavioral changes to record the different kinds of experiences that stimulate contiguity between stimulus and response.  Most innate behaviors (e.g., laughing, crying, and defensiveness) are not acquired through the learning process; however, there are different kinds of conditioning that does stimulate identifiable learning.        
Types of Learning: Conditioning
     Through scientific research, theorists have found that changes in behavior (learning) are derived from experience.  There are two forms of conditioning that exemplify behavioral modification; they are instrumental and classical conditioning.  Classical conditioning can best be explained through Ivan Pavlov’s research; whereby he took an unconditioned stimulus (US), associated it with a controlled stimulus (CS), turning an uncontrolled response (UR) into a conditioned response (CR) (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009).  One such example of classical conditioning can be demonstrated as a result of someone becoming ill after eating tainted food.  A visit to the same eatery can induce the same symptoms of the previous illness (e.g., nausea and vomiting) even though no additional foods have been ingested. 
     With instrumental or more commonly called operant conditioning, reinforcement of an existing behavior is “instrumental” to a desired behavior.  B. F. Skinner (1904-1990) is well known for his operant conditioning methods; whereby he takes “a Plexiglas cage with a metal-grid floor and food lever” to reinforce a lever pressing action for the caged rat to receive food  (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009, p. 8).  His experiments also reinforce escape and avoidance behaviors when paired with an electrical shock.  In this example, subjects receive their reward by modifying their behavior to terminate or avoid the shock stimulus.  Although there are other methods that produce learning, “many theorists believe that complex behavior ultimately can be understood in terms of classical or instrumental conditioning” (p. 8). 
The Relationship between Learning and Cognition
     Only through the observation of behavior can theorists continue to identify the relationship between learning and cognition.  According to Olson and Hergenhahn (2009) naturalistic observation is only a first step to the study of learning; eventually a systematic approach must be taken.  Therefore, the study of learning from different paradigms is essential to identifying the relationship between learning and cognition.  Through associationism (from the laws of association by Aristotle), “because a child hears ‘dog’, sees the dog, and feels the dog at the same time, these sensations come together as the idea of a dog” (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009, p. 31).  With additional cognitive experiences that same child will begin to expand on his or her mindset of dog to other animals with similar identifiable traits (e.g., four legs, fur, and a tail).  However, certain aspects of this theory also lay within other paradigms.
     As can be seen, cognition is the mental processes that enable learning, while intervening variables are what change behavior, and stimulate learning.  Although these variables or experiences are many, the two most frequently cited are classical and instrumental conditioning, which many theorists claim explain the majority of how all learning takes place.  However, the more one understands about learning and cognition, the greater will be the understanding of not only “normal and adaptive behavior but also… [that of] maladaptive and abnormal behavior” (Olson & Hergenhahn, 2009, pp. 9-10).




 

No comments:

Post a Comment