Sunday, October 19, 2014

Cognitive Psychology Definition


PSY/360
     Knowing is the key underpinning of cognition.  Norbert Wiener, Edward Tolman, George Miller, and Ulrich Neisser were instrumental in the development of cognitive psychology; although, “people have been thinking about how the mind works for more than 2,400 years” (Willingham, 2007, p. 5).  Dissatisfied with the behaviorist perspective of reasoning these men turned to the mental processes in which people perceive, think, learn, memorize, and use information.  Turning from the observable to the unobservable, however, created its own difficulties (e.g., how does one prove what is perceived and not seen?).  Time is the answer; history has lent itself to the development of cognitive psychology and the observation of the unobservable.
Milestones in the Development of Cognitive Psychology
Philosophical Underpinnings
     Philosophers have pondered on such topics that involve the relationship between the material brain, the conscious mind, and moral motivations for over 2,400 years (Chiong, 2011; Willingham, 2007).  In trying to gain access on how perception, memory, and the origin (nature or nurture) of knowledge are attained, their assumptions, not their questions became the springboard upon which psychology emerged; questioning what can and cannot be explained.  Such philosophical underpinnings are the foundations of the technology that influence society even today.
The Advent of the Scientific Method
     As the age of empirical investigation advance, scientists’ became unsatisfied with only philosophical underpinnings and began to demand scientific proof to such theories of how people think and respond.  A logical process that can be observed and contemplated; with this method, Willingham (2007) explains, “The world can be understood and predicted” (p. 10).  Although this is a very deterministic point of view, it seems reasonable until one tries to apply it to studying the mind.  However, the struggles of trying to apply such scientific methods to the workings of the mind ultimately launched modern psychology.
The Rise and Fall of Behaviorism
     At a time when structured, observable science was in place, William Wundt (1832-1920) pursued, published, taught, wrote, and pushed for his adjenda; psychology – the study of the mind. Wundt’s method of study became known as introspectionism; however, in 1913 Watson challenged introspectionists’ assumptions that they could account for mental processes.  Instead he offered behaviorism; “the study of human psychology through the examination and analysis of objectively observable and quantifiable behavioral events” (behaviorism, n.d., para 1).  Although behaviorism provided the framework for understanding how learning takes place, it did not answer the underlying concerns of how human mental processes (e.g., language, and memory) function.   
The Age of the Computer
     Not until the dawning of the computer age, with its capacity to take information, store it, and retrieve it for later use, did such metaphors for the human mind ignite.  This seemed to be the answer for mental human processes also.  Believing that humans use representations and processes, new assumptions were surfacing around this “information processing model” (Willingham, 2007, p. 28).  These assumptions, suggests Willingham (2007), are that thought and behavior are a process, objects and events (sights and sounds) underlie processing, and information processing (interpretation of events) is completed in organized stages (retrieved from memory and expressed through speech and behavior).  This line of thought was useful to opening the way for further study in the area of cognitive psychology.
Importance of Behavioral Observation in Cognitive Psychology
     Neisser (1978) once quoted “If X is an interesting or socially important aspect of memory, then psychologists have hardly ever studied X” (as restated by Hyman, 2012, para. 6).  With the assumptions that there is no way to observe directly mental processes, one must infer the processes as a direct result of one’s behavior (Willingham, 2007).  Such behavioral observation allows psychologists to test and evaluate theories and assumptions; however, concerning cognitive psychology unobservable abstract constructs can be used; such as those used in both computer science and neuroscience.  Taking what is known and combining with inferences, a hypothesis can be drawn and tested for outcome.  This process brings cognitive psychology back into the scientific realm.
     History is a good indicator of how man’s mental processes continue to grow and expand.  From ancient philosophers it is learned that it is not what, but how someone goes about obtaining information that become the foundations that influence change in society.  However, even logical processes, such as the scientific method may not be able to uncover every truth.  Behaviorism, for example, once again took psychology away from the constructs of the mind to an observable behavioral evaluation, even though much was learned about how behaviors are formed, it did not explain the internal mental processes to produce said behaviors.  It was not until psychologists metaphorically compared computer processes to human mental processes that cognitive psychology achieved its largest advances.  This, however, did incorporate the knowledge of observation and the scientific method, brining cognitive psychology back into the sciences.  The future of cognitive psychology is bright as it advances into the neurosciences and perhaps back to its philosophical roots.        

 

No comments:

Post a Comment